http://apologeticspress.org/APContent.aspx?category=12&article=1351
The Origin of Language and Communication
[EDITOR’S NOTE: This is the first article by our
newest staff member, Dr. Dave Miller, who joins us this month as the
chairman of our recently established Department of New Testament
Studies. Dave’s doctorate is in speech communications, which makes him
especially qualified to write on the topic of the origin of language and
communication. See my “
Note from the Editor” for an introduction to this talented addition to our staff.]
In 1994, an article appeared in
Time magazine titled “How Man
Began.” Within that article was the following bold assertion: “No
single, essential difference separates human beings from other animals”
(Lemonick, 143[11]:81). Yet, in what is obviously a contradiction to
such a statement, all evolutionists admit that communication via speech
is uniquely human—so much so that it often is used as the singular, and
most important, dividing line between humans and animals. In his book
titled
Eve Spoke, evolutionist Philip Lieberman admitted:
Speech is so essential to our concept of intelligence that its
possession is virtually equated with being human. Animals who talk are human, because what sets us apart from other animals is the “gift” of speech (1998, p. 5, emp. in orig.).
In
The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Human Evolution, editors Jones, Martin, and Pilbeam conceded that “
[t]here are no non-human languages,”
and then went on to observe that “language is an adaptation unique to
humans, and yet the nature of its uniqueness and its biological basis
are notoriously difficult to define” (1992, p. 128, emp. added). In his
book,
The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain, Terrance Deacon noted:
In this context, then, consider the case of human language. It is one
of the most distinctive behavioral adaptations on the planet. Languages
evolved in only one species, in only one way, without precedent, except
in the most general sense. And the differences between languages and all
other natural modes of communicating are vast (1997, p. 25).
What events transpired that have allowed humans to speak, while animals
remain silent? If we are to believe the evolutionary teaching currently
taking place in colleges and universities around the world, speech
evolved as a natural process over time. Yet no one is quite sure how,
and there are no known animals that are in a transition phase from
non-speaking to speaking. In fact, in the
Atlas of Languages,
this remarkable admission can be found: “No languageless community has
ever been found” (Matthews, et al., 1996, p. 7). This represents no
small problem for evolution.
In fact, the origin of speech and language (along with the development
of sex and reproduction) remains one of the most significant hurdles in
evolutionary theory, even in the twenty-first century. In fact, many
evolutionists simply have stopped discussing the matter completely. Jean
Aitchison noted:
In 1866, a ban on the topic was incorporated into the founding statues
of the Linguistic Society of Paris, perhaps the foremost academic
linguistic institution of the time: “The Society does not accept papers
on either the origin of language or the invention of a universal
language” (2000, p. 5).
That is an amazing (albeit inadvertent) admission of defeat, especially
coming from a group of such eminent scientists, researchers, and
scholars.
The truth of the matter is, however, that the origin of human languages
can be discerned—
but not via the theory of evolution.
We invite your attention to the discussion that follows, which
demonstrates conclusively that humans were created by God with the
unique ability to employ speech for communication.
EVOLUTIONARY THEORIES ON
THE ORIGIN OF SPEECH
Many animals are capable of using sounds to communicate. However, there
is a colossal difference between the hoot of an owl or the grunt of a
pig, and a human standing before an audience reciting Robert Frost’s
“The Road Not Taken.” This enormous chasm between humans and animals has
led to a multiplicity of theories on exactly how man came upon this
unequaled capability. But there is a single, common theme that stands
out amidst all the theories: “
The world’s languages evolved spontaneously. They were not designed” (Deacon, p. 110, emp. added).
Design implies a Designer; thus, evolutionists have conjured up
theories that consider language nothing more than a fortuitous chain of
events. Most of these theories involve humans growing bigger brains,
which then made it physiologically possible for people to develop speech
and language. For instance, in the foreword of her book,
The Seeds of Speech, Jean Aitchison hypothesized:
Physically, a deprived physical environment led to more meat-eating
and, as a result, a bigger brain. The enlarged brain led to the
premature birth of humans, and in consequence a protracted childhood,
during which mothers cooed and crooned to their offspring. An upright
stance altered the shape of the mouth and vocal tract, allowing a range
of coherent sounds to be uttered (2000, p. x).
Thus, according to Aitchison, we can thank “a deprived physical
environment” for our ability to talk and communicate. Another
evolutionist, John McCrone, put it this way:
It all started with an ape that learned to speak. Man’s hominid
ancestors were doing well enough, even though the world had slipped into
the cold grip of the ice ages. They had solved a few key problems that
had held back the other branches of the ape family, such as how to find
enough food to feed their rather oversized brains. Then man’s ancestors
happened on the trick of language. Suddenly, a whole new mental
landscape opened up. Man became self-aware and self-possessed (1991, p.
9).
Question: How (and why) did that first ape learn to speak? It is easy
to assert that “it all started with an ape that learned to speak.” But
it is much more difficult to describe
how this took place, especially in light of our failure to teach apes to speak today. In his book,
From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language, Michael Corballis stated:
My own view is that language developed much more gradually, starting
with the gestures of apes, then gathering momentum as the bipedal
hominins evolved. The appearance of the larger-brained genus Homo
some 2 million years ago may have signaled the emergence and later
development of syntax, with vocalizations providing a mounting refrain.
What may have distinguished Homo sapiens was the final switch
from a mixture of gestural and vocal communication to an autonomous
vocal language, embellished by gesture but not dependent on it (2002, p.
183).
The truth however, is that evolutionists can only speculate as to the
origin of language. Evolutionist Carl Zimmer summed it up well when he
wrote:
No one knows the exact chronology of this evolution, because language
leaves precious few traces on the human skeleton. The voice box is a
flimsy piece of cartilage that rots away. It is suspended from a slender
C-shaped bone called a hyoid, but the ravages of time usually destroy
the hyoid too (2001, p. 291).
Thus, theories are plentiful—while the evidence to support those
theories remains mysteriously unavailable. Add to this the fact that
humans acquire the ability to communicate (and even learn some of the
basic rules of syntax) by the age of two, and you begin to see why
Aitchison admitted:
Of course, holes still remain in our knowledge: in particular, at what
stage did language leap from being something new which humans discovered
to being something which every newborn human is scheduled to acquire?
This is still a puzzle (p. ix).
A “puzzle” indeed!
ADAM—THE FIRST HUMAN
TO TALK AND COMMUNICATE
In a chapter he titled “What, When, and Where did Eve Speak to Adam and He to Her?,” Philip Lieberman commented:
In the five-million-year-long lineage that connects us to the common
ancestors of apes and human beings, there have been many Adams and many
Eves. In the beginning was the word, but the vocal communications of our
most distant hominid ancestors five million years or so ago probably
didn’t really differ from those of the ape-hominid ancestor (1998, p.
133).
Using biblical terminology, Lieberman had written a year earlier: “For
with speech came a capacity for thought that had never existed before,
and that has transformed the world. In the beginning was the word”
(1997, p. 27).
When God created the first human beings—Adam and Eve—He created them in
His own image (Genesis 1:26-27). This likeness unquestionably included
the ability to engage in intelligible speech via human language. In
fact, God spoke to them from the very beginning of their existence as
humans (Genesis 1:28-30). Hence, they possessed the ability to
understand verbal communication—
and to speak themselves!
God gave very specific instructions to the man
before the woman was even created (Genesis 2:15-17). Adam gave names to the animals
before
the creation of Eve (Genesis 2:19-20). Since both the man and the woman
were created on the sixth day, the creation of the man preceded the
creation of the woman by only hours. So,
Adam had the ability to speak on the very day that he was brought into existence!
That same day, God put Adam to sleep and performed history’s first
human surgery. He fashioned the female of the species from a portion of
the male’s body. God then presented the woman to the man (no doubt in
what we would refer to as the first marriage ceremony). Observe Adam’s
response: “And Adam said, ‘This is now bone of my bones and flesh of my
flesh; she shall be called Woman, because she was taken out of man’ ”
(Genesis 2:23). Here is Adam—less than twenty-four hours
old—articulating intelligible speech with a well-developed vocabulary
and advanced powers of expression. Note also that Eve engaged in
intelligent conversation with Satan (Genesis 3:1-5). An unbiased
observer is forced to conclude that Adam and Eve were
created by God
with oral communication capability. Little wonder, then, that God said
to Moses: “Who had made man’s mouth?... Have not I, the Lord? Now
therefore, go, and I will be with your mouth and teach you what you
shall say” (Exodus 4:11-12).
This circumstance should not surprise us, since the rest of the created
order also was brought into existence fully formed and operational.
Adam’s body was that of a man—not a child. His body possessed
reproductive capability (Genesis 1:28). His mind was mentally and
psychologically functional on the level of an adult. Likewise, trees and
plants were completely operational in their photosynthetic,
reproductive, and fruit-bearing capability (Genesis 1:11-12). Animals,
too, were created fully functional (Genesis 1:20-25). And, the Sun,
Moon, planets, and stars were created instantaneously to provide the
services they were intended to provide (Genesis 1:14-18). Once again,
the biblical explanation of the beginning of the human race and
linguistic functionality is logical, reasonable, and scientifically
feasible. The evolutionary model is not.
THE TOWER OF BABEL—
AND UNIVERSAL LANGUAGE
Nobody knows exactly how many languages there are in the world, partly
because of the difficulty of distinguishing between a language and a
sub-language (or dialects within it). But those who have tried to count
usually end up around the 5,000 mark (Aitchison, 2000, p. 26).
The Bible’s explanation of the origin of multiple human languages is
provided in the Tower of Babel incident recorded in Genesis 11:1-9.
Scripture simply and confidently asserts: “Now the whole earth had one
language and one speech” (11:1). When Noah and his family stepped off
the ark, they spoke a single language that was passed on to their
offspring. As the population increased, it apparently remained localized
in a single geographical region. Consequently, little or no linguistic
variation ensued. But when a generation defiantly rejected God’s
instructions to scatter over the planet, God miraculously intervened and
initiated the major language groupings of the human race. This action
forced the population to proceed with God’s original intention to
inhabit the Earth (cf. Isaiah 45:18) by clustering according to shared
languages.
This depiction of the origin of languages coincides with the present
status of these languages. The available linguistic evidence does not
support the model postulated by evolutionary sources for the origin of
languages. Evolutionary linguists believe that all human languages have
descended from a single, primitive language, which itself evolved from
the grunts and noises of the lower animals. The single most influential
“hopeful monster” theory of the evolution of human language was proposed
by the famous linguist from MIT, Noam Chomsky,
and has since been echoed by numerous linguists, philosophers,
anthropologists, and psychologists. Chomsky argued that the innate
ability of children to acquire the grammar necessary for a language can
be explained only if one assumes that all grammars are variations of a
single, generic “universal grammar,” and that all human brains come
“with a built-in language organ that contains this language blueprint”
(Deacon, 1997, p. 35).
Explaining this “innate ability,” a “universal grammar,” and the
“built-in language organ” of humans has proven to be, well, impossible!
Steven Pinker, the eminent psychologist also of MIT, candidly lamented this very fact in his best-selling book,
How the Mind Works. In addressing the failure of “our species’ ” scientists to solve these types of plaguing, perennial problems, he wrote:
[T]he species’ best minds have flung themselves at the puzzles for
millennia but have made no progress in solving them. Another is that
they have a different character from even the most challenging problems
of science. Problems such as how a child learns language or how a fertilized egg becomes an organism are horrendous in practice and may never be solved completely (1997a, p. 562, emp. added).
However, the existing state of human language nevertheless suggests
that the variety of dialects and sub-languages has developed from a
relatively few (perhaps even less than twenty) languages. These original
“proto-languages”—from which all others allegedly have developed—were
distinct within themselves, with no previous ancestral language.
Creationist Carl Wieland rightly remarked: “The evidence is wonderfully
consistent with the notion that a small number of languages, separately
created at Babel, has diversified into the huge variety of languages we
have today” (1999, p. 22).
THE BRAIN’S LANGUAGE CENTERS—
CREATED BY GOD
In contemplating how language arose, evolutionists frequently link the
development of the brain to the appearance of languages. But when one
considers that more than 5,000 languages exist, it is incomprehensible
to suggest that the invention of language could be viewed as some sort
of simple, clear-cut addition to human physiology made possible by an
enlarged brain unique to
Homo sapiens. Terrance Deacon commented on the intricacy of evolving a language when he said:
For a language feature to have such an impact on brain evolution that
all members of the species come to share it, it must remain invariable
across even the most drastic language change possible (p. 329, emp. in orig.).
|
Left hemisphere of human brain with language centers—Brocas’s area and Wernicke’s area— highlighted |
The complexity underlying speech first revealed itself in patients who
were suffering various communication problems. Researchers began
noticing analogous responses among patients with similar injuries. The
ancient Greeks noticed that brain damage could cause the loss of the
ability to speak (a condition known as aphasia). Centuries later, in
1836, Marc Dax described a group of patients that could not speak
normally. Dax reported that all of these patients experienced damage to
the left hemisphere of their brain. In 1861, Paul Broca described a
patient who could utter only a single word—“tan.” When this patient
died, Broca examined his brain and observed significant damage to the
left frontal cortex, which has since become known anatomically as
“Broca’s area.”
While patients with damage to Broca’s area can understand language, they generally are unable to produce speech because words are not formed properly, thus slurring their speech.
In 1876, Carl Wernicke discovered that language problems also could
result from damage to another section of the brain. This area, later
termed “Wernicke’s area,” is located in the posterior part of the
temporal lobe.
Damage to Wernicke’s area results in a loss of the ability to understand language.
Thus, patients can continue to speak, but the words are put together in
such a way that they make no sense. Interestingly, in most people
(approximately 97%) both Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area are found only
in the left hemisphere, which explains the language deficits observed
in patients with brain damage to the left side of the brain.
Evolutionists freely acknowledge that
[t]he relationship between brain size and language is unclear.
Possibly, increased social interaction combined with tactical deception
gave the brain an initial impetus. Better nourishment due to meat-eating
may also have played a part. Then brain size and language possibly
increased together (Aitchison, 2000, p. 85).
However, the human brain is not simply larger. The connections are
vastly different as well. As Deacon admitted: “Looking more closely, we
will discover that a radical re-engineering of the whole brain has taken
place, and on a scale that is unprecedented” (p. 45). In order to speak
a word that has been read, information is obtained from the eyes and
travels to the visual cortex. From the primary visual cortex,
information is transmitted to the posterior speech area (which includes
Wernicke’s area). From there, information travels to Broca’s area, and
then to the primary motor cortex to provide the necessary muscle
contractions to produce the sound. To speak a word that has been heard,
we must invoke the primary
auditory cortex, not the
visual cortex. Deacon commented on this complex neuronal network—
which does not occur in animals—when he wrote:
There is, without doubt, something special about human brains that
enables us to do with ease what no other species can do even minimally
without intense effort and remarkably insightful training. We not only
have the ability to create and easily learn simple symbol systems such
as the chimps Sherman and Austin struggled to learn, but in learning
languages we acquire an immensely complex rule system and a rich
vocabulary at a time in our lives when it is otherwise very difficult to
learn even elementary arithmetic. Many a treatise on grammatical theory
has failed to provide an adequate accounting of the implicit knowledge
that even a four-year-old appears to possess about her newly acquired
language (p. 103).
ANATOMY OF SPEECH
The specific mechanics involved in speaking have anatomical requirements that are found
only in humans.
There is no animal living presently, nor has one been observed in the
fossil record, that possesses anything close to the “voice box” (as we
commonly call it) present in humans. As information scientist Werner
Gitt observed in his fascinating book,
The Wonder of Man:
Only man has the gift of speech, a characteristic otherwise only
possessed by God. This separates us clearly from the animal kingdom....
In addition to the necessary “software” for speech, we have also been
provided with the required “hardware” (1999, p. 101).
Furthermore, the complete lack of any “transitional” animal form (with
the requisite speech hardware) in the fossil record poses a significant
continuity problem for evolutionists. As Deacon noted:
This lack of precedent makes language a problem for biologists.
Evolutionary explanations are about biological continuity, so a lack of
continuity limits the use of the comparative method in several important
ways. We can’t ask, “What ecological variable correlates with
increasing language use in a sample species?” Nor can we investigate the
“neurological correlates of increased language complexity.” There is no
range of species to include in our analysis (p. 34).
To simplify the anatomy required for human speech by using an analogy,
think of a small tube resting inside a larger tube. The inner tube
consists of the trachea going down to the lungs, and the larynx (which
houses the voice box). At the larynx, the inner tube opens out to the
larger tube, which is known as the pharynx. It carries only sound
up to the mouth, but it also carries food and water from the mouth
down
to the stomach. A rather simplistic description of how humans utter
sounds in speech can be characterized by the control of air generated by
the lungs, flowing through the vocal tract, vibrating over the vocal
cord, filtered by facial muscle activity, and released out of the mouth
and nose. Just as sound is generated from blowing air across the narrow
mouth of a bottle, air is passed over the vocal cords, which can be
tightened or relaxed to produce various resonances.
The physiological components necessary can be divided into: (1)
supralaryngeal vocal tract; (2) larynx; and (3) subglottal system (see
illustration at below). In 1848, Johannes Muller demonstrated that human
speech involved the modulation of acoustic energy by the airway above
the larynx (referred to as the supralaryngeal tract). Sound energy for
speech is generated in the larynx at the vocal folds. The subglottal
system—which consists of the lungs, trachea, and their associated
muscles—provides the necessary power for speech production. The lungs
produce the initial air pressure that is essential for the speech
signal; the pharyngeal cavity, oral cavity, and nasal cavity shape the
final output sound that is perceived as speech.
BIRDS OF A FEATHER—OR NAKED APE?
Imagine the conundrum in which evolutionists find themselves when it
comes to speech and language. The animal that comes closest to producing
anything that even vaguely resembles human speech is not another
primate, but rather a bird. Deacon observed:
In fact, most birds easily outshine any mammal in vocal skills, and
though dogs, cats, horses, and monkeys are remarkably capable learners
in many domains, vocalization is not one of them. Our remarkable vocal
abilities are not part of a trend, but an exception (pp. 30-31).
For instance, a famous African gray parrot in England named Toto can
pronounce words so clearly that he sounds rather human. Like humans,
birds can produce fluent, complex sounds. We both share a
double-barreled, double-layered system involving tunes and dialects—a
system controlled by the left side of our brains. And just like young
children, juvenile birds experience a period termed “sub-song” where
they twitter in what resembles the babbling of a young child learning to
speak. Yet Toto does not have a “language” as humans understand it.
Humans use language for many more purposes than birds use song.
Consider, too, that it is mostly male birds that sing. Females remain
songless unless they are injected with the male hormone testosterone
(see Nottebohm, 1980). Also consider that humans frequently communicate
intimately between two or three people, while bird communication is a
fairly long-distance affair.
One of the big “success” stories in looking at the human-like qualities
of non-human primates is a male bonobo chimpanzee known as Kanzi (see
Savage-Rumbaugh and Lewin, 1994; Skoyles and Sagan, 2002, pp. 217-220).
Kanzi was born October 28, 1990, and began his long journey to learn to
“speak” as a result of the training provided for his mother, Matata, via
a “talking” keyboard. Matata never did master the keyboard, but Kanzi
did. Through many years of intense training and close social contact
with humans, this remarkable animal attained the language abilities of
an average two-year-old human. By age ten, he had a “spoken” vocabulary
(via the keyboard) of some two hundred words. In fact, Kanzi was able to
go beyond the mere parroting or “aping” of humans; he actually could
communicate his wants and needs, express feelings, and use tools.
Inasmuch as Kanzi could accomplish such things, does this prove that
chimps are merely hairy, child-like versions of humans?
Hardly. To use the words of the famous American news commentator, Paul
Harvey, someone needs to tell “the rest of the story.” For example, in
their 2002 volume,
Up from Dragons, John Skoyles and Dorion Sagan discussed Kanzi at great length. Among other things, they wrote:
Kanzi shows that while chimps may have the potential to learn language,
they require a “gifted” environment to do so. Kanzi was surrounded by
intelligent apes with PhDs [i.e., humans—DM/BH/BT]
who spoke to him and gave him a stream of rich interactions. They gave
Kanzi’s brain a world in which it could play at developing its ability
to communicate.... Therefore, as much as in his brain, Kanzi’s skill lies in the environment that helped shape it (pp. 215,216, emp. added).
Kanzi does not possess the anatomical equipment required for speech.
Truth be told, no animal does. As Skoyles and Sagan went on to note:
“Chimps lack the vocal abilities needed for making speech sounds—speech
requires a skilled coordination between breathing and making movements
with the larynx that chimps lack” (p. 214). Humans, however,
do possess the anatomical equipment required for speech.
But there is more. Regardless of how much instruction such animals
receive, there appear to be built-in limits on their progress. On
February 15, 1994, the public television program NOVA
aired the show titled “Can Chimps Talk?” (for a full transcript of the
show go to www.primate.wisc.edu/pin/nova/html). The show began with a
“conversation” with Kanzi, who was required to use a talking keyboard to
respond to queries from his human counterpart. As the television
program demonstrated quite effectively, Kanzi frequently responded
incorrectly when asked a question. For instance, one of the humans
asked, “Is there any other food you’d like me to bring in the backpack?”
Kanzi’s talking keyboard response was: “ball.”
The program then focused on Washoe, a chimpanzee that, in the 1970s,
was taught a portion of American Sign Language by Allen and Beatrice
Gardner at the University of Nevada. By the time Washoe was five, the
trainers reported that she could use 133 signs. Headlines were quick to
report that a
non-human primate was using
human language.
This spurred other scientists, such as Herb Terrace, to begin
experimenting with animal language. Terrace set out to replicate some of
the Gardners’ study by using his own ape, Nim Chimsky (sarcastically
named after MIT scientist Noam Chomsky, who
believes language is confined strictly to humans). The main goal of the
project was to determine if a chimpanzee could create a sentence. In the
documentary, Terrace stated: “I have concluded that, unfortunately, the
answer to that question is no.” Nim’s sign usage could best be
interpreted as a series of “conditioned discriminations” similar to
behaviors seen in many less-intelligent animals. This work suggested
that Nim, like circus animals, was using words only to obtain food
rewards. Terrace realized that while Nim seemed to be using a
combination of signs, he actually was imitating the trainer. This
prompted Terrace to examine some of the Gardners’ films. He decided that
Washoe, too, was being led by his teacher and was merely imitating.
As Skoyles and Sagan candidly admitted, Kanzi’s skill was “in the
environment that helped shape it.” That is precisely what Terrace
discovered. Such an assessment always will be true of “talking animals.”
But it is not always true of humans! Consider the following case in point.
As we mentioned earlier, the eminent linguist Noam Chomsky has
championed the idea that humans are born with a built-in “universal
grammar”—a series of biological switches for complex language that is
set in place in the early years of childhood. This, he believes, is why
children can grasp elaborate language rules, even at an early age—
without adults to teach them.
Powerful support for Chomsky’s theory emerged from a decade-long study
of 500 deaf children in Managua, Nicaragua, which was reported in the
December 1995 issue of
Scientific American (Horgan, 1995,
273[6]:18-19). These children started attending special schools in 1979,
but none used or was taught a formal sign language. Within a few years,
and under no direction from teachers or other adults, they began to
develop a basic “pidgin” sign language. This quickly was modified by
younger children entering school, with the current version taking on a
complex and consistent grammar. If Chomsky is correct, where, then, did
humans get their innate ability for language? Chomsky himself will not
even hazard a guess. In his opinion, “very few people are concerned with
the origin of language because most consider it a hopeless question”
(as quoted in Ross, 1991, 264[4]:146). The development of language, he
admits, is a “mystery.” The fundamental failing of naturalistic theories
is that they are inadequate to explain the origins of something so
complex and information-rich as human language, which itself is a gift
of God and part of man’s having been created “in His image” (see Lyons
and Thompson, 2002).
The fact is, no animal is capable of speaking in the manner in which people can speak. Speech is a peculiarly
human trait. Steven Pinker, director of MIT’s Center of Cognitive Neuroscience, stated in
The Language Instinct: The New Science of Language and Mind:
As you are reading these words, you are taking part in one of the
wonders of the natural world. For you and I belong to a species with a
remarkable ability: we can shape events in each other’s brains with
remarkable precision. I am not referring to telepathy or mind control or
the other obsessions of fringe science; even in the depictions of
believers, these are blunt instruments compared to an ability that is
uncontroversially present in every one of us. That ability is language.
Simply by making noises with our mouths, we can reliably cause precise
new combinations of ideas to arise in each other’s minds. The ability
comes so naturally that we are apt to forget what a miracle it is.... [H]uman language is based on a very different design.... Even the seat of human language in the brain is special... (1997b, pp. 1,365, emp. added).
Without detracting anything from primates like Kanzi and Washoe,
fundamental differences between animals and humans nevertheless remain.
Unlike human children, animals: (1) do not have a special region in the
brain devoted to language; (2) possess a much smaller brain overall; and
(3) lack the anatomy to speak the words they may think. In summary,
humans have an innate, built-in, hard-wired ability to acquire and
communicate complex language from the moment of their birth. Animals do
not. Admittedly, animals
do possess a measure of understanding.
They can learn to respond to commands and signs, and in some instances
even can be trained to use minimal portions of human sign language. But
even though apes, dogs, and birds can be trained to do certain things,
they cannot reason and communicate ideas with others so as to have true
mental communion. Why? The intelligence of animals is, quite bluntly,
unlike that of humankind. As biologist John N. Moore observed:
Animals can think in several ways...though only on the perceptual, not
on the conceptual level. The key difference here is one between
conceptual and perceptual thinking. The latter, which is typical of
animal thinking, requires the actual or nearly immediate presence of the
pertinent objects. Man’s thinking, on the other hand, is independent of
the presence of pertinent objects. It is, in fact, independent of
objects altogether, as is the case with logical or mathematical
exercises. Secondly, the difference between human and animal thinking
resides in the fact that, whether or not the object of the mental
operation is present, animals cannot make judgments or engage in
reasoning. For example, animals are unable to conclude that such and
such is or is not the case in a given situation or that if such and such is the case, then so and so is not (1983, p. 344, emp. and ellipses in orig.).
The issue is not “can animals think?,” but rather “can they think the
way humans do?” The answer, obviously, is a resounding “No!” Although
animal trainers and investigators since the seventeenth century have
tried to teach chimpanzees to talk, no chimpanzee has ever managed it. A
chimpanzee’s sound-producing anatomy is simply too different from that
of humans. Chimpanzees might be able to produce a muffled approximation
of human speech—if their brains could plan and execute the necessary
articulate maneuvers. But to do this, they would have to have our
brains, which they obviously do not (see Lieberman, 1997, p. 27).
COMPLEXITY OF LANGUAGE—
UNIQUELY HUMAN
No known language in the whole of human history can be considered “primitive” in any sense of the word. In her book,
What is Linguistics?, Suzette Elgin wrote:
The most ancient languages for which we have written texts—Sanskrit for
example—are often far more intricate and complicated in their
grammatical forms than many other contemporary languages (1973, p. 44).
Lewis Thomas, a distinguished physician, scientist, and longtime
director and chancellor of the Sloan Kettering Cancer Center in
Manhattan, acknowledged: “...Language is so incomprehensible a problem
that the language we use for discussing the matter is itself becoming
incomprehensible” (1980, p. 59). It appears that, from the beginning,
human communication was
designed with a tremendous amount of
complexity and forethought, and has allowed us to communicate not only
with one another, but also with the
Designer of language.
In a paper titled “Evolution of Universal Grammar” that appeared in the January 2001 issue of
Science,
M.A. Nowak and his colleagues attempted to discount the gulf that
separates human and animals (Nowak, et al., 2001). This paper, which was
a continuation of a 1999 paper titled “The Evolution of Language”
(Nowak and Krakauer, 1999), used mathematical calculations in an effort
to predict the evolution of grammar and the rules surrounding it. While
Nowak and his team inferred that the evolution of universal grammar can
occur via natural selection, they freely admitted that “
the question concerning why only humans evolved language is hard to answer”
(1999, 96:8031, emp. added). Hard to answer indeed! The mathematical
models presented in these papers do not tell us anything about the
origination of the multitude of languages used in the world today. If
man truly did evolve from an ape-like ancestor, how did the phonologic
[the branch of linguistics that deals with the sounds of speech and
their production] component of our languages become so diverse and
variegated? Nowak’s paper also did not clarify the origination of
written languages, or describe how the language process was initiated in
the first humans, considering we know today that parents teach
languages to their offspring.
Nowak and his collaborators believe that the “first step” in the
evolution of language was “signal-object associations.” They speculate
that common objects, frequently utilized, were given a representative
signal or sign (in a manner similar to modern sign language). These
researchers also believe that early in evolution, these signals were
“likely to have been noisy” and therefore “mistaken for each other.”
Nowak suggests that these errors necessitated the formation of words,
and describes this step in the evolution of language as going “from an
analogue to a digital system.” However, there is no evidence that
demonstrates how these “prehistoric” people made the quantum leap from
signals to words. The last step Nowak describes is the evolution of
basic grammatical rules in an effort to convey even more information
than just simple words. While these speculations make a nice, neat,
progressive path toward human language, they do little to explain
adequately the anatomical differences found in animals and humans. The
human supralaryngeal airway differs from that of any other adult mammal,
and is essential for speech. While it is true that chimpanzees have
been taught to communicate by means of sign language, they cannot speak,
and do not appear to use any complex syntax in communication.
Nowak and his colleagues began with the assumption that language
“evolved as a means of communicating information between individuals”
(1999, 96:8030), and then went on to speculate that natural selection
favors the emergence of a universal, rule-based language system. But if
it is true that natural selection “favors” a complex language, how do we
account for the non-vocal communication observed in animals, and why
hasn’t this communication “emerged” into a formal language in those
animals? In an effort to explain this embarrassing lack of
understanding, Nowak, et al. offered several speculations as to why
animals have not evolved a better form of communication. In their
explanation, they listed the following:
-
Signal-object associations form only when information transfer is beneficial to both speaker and listener.
-
In the presence of errors, only a very limited communication system
describing a small number of objects can evolve by natural selection.
-
Although grammar can be an advantage for small systems, it may be necessary only if the language refers to many events.
-
Thus, animals may not possess the need to describe “many” events.
But such speculations leave gaping holes in regard to potential
explanations as to why animals cannot use speech. As Deacon noted:
How could anyone doubt that language complexity is the problem?
Languages are indeed complicated things. They are probably orders of
magnitude more complicated than the next-most-complicated communication
system outside of the human sphere. And they are indeed almost
impossibly difficult for other species to acquire (1997, p. 40).
Also, consider that when language first appears on the scene, it
already is fully developed and very complex. The late Harvard
paleontologist George Gaylord Simpson described it this way:
Even the peoples with least complex cultures have highly sophisticated
languages, with complex grammar and large vocabularies, capable of
naming and discussing anything that occurs in the sphere occupied by
their speakers. The oldest language that can be reconstructed is already
modern, sophisticated, complete from an evolutionary point of view
(1966, p. 477).
Chomsky summed it up well when he stated:
Human language appears to be a unique phenomenon, without significant
analogue in the animal world.... There is no reason to suppose that the
“gaps” are bridgeable. There is no more of a basis for assuming an
evolutionary development from breathing to walking (1972, pp. 67-68).
CONCLUSION
The fact of the matter is that language is quintessentially a human
trait. All attempts to shed light on the evolution of human language
have failed—due to the lack of knowledge regarding the origin of
any
language, and due to the lack of an animal that possesses any
“transitional” form of communication. This leaves evolutionists with a
huge gulf to bridge between humans with their innate communication
abilities, and the grunts, barks, or chatterings of animals. Deacon
lamented:
So this is the real mystery. Even under these loosened criteria, there
are no simple languages used among other species, though there are many
other equally or more complicated modes of communication. Why not? And
the problem is even more counterintuitive when we consider the almost
insurmountable difficulties of teaching language to other species. This
is surprising, because there are many clever species. Though researchers
report that language-like communication has been taught to nonhuman
species, even the best results are not above legitimate challenges, and
the fact that it is difficult to prove whether or not some of these
efforts have succeeded attests to the rather limited scope of the
resulting behaviors, as well as to deep disagreements about what exactly
constitutes language-like behavior (p. 41).
Another scholar who recognized this chasm between humans and animals commented:
The very fact...that human animals are ready to engage in a great
“garrulity” over the merits and demerits of essentially unprovable
hypotheses, is an exciting testimony to the gap between humans and other
animals (Holloway, 1976, 280:330).
Gap indeed! Humans are capable of communicating in human language
because God created them with the ability to do so! The Bible still
offers the only plausible explanation for the origin of human language
when it records: “Then God said, ‘Let Us make man in Our image,
according to Our likeness;’...So God created man in His own image; in
the image of God He created him; male and female He created them”
(Genesis 1:26-27).
REFERENCES
Aitchson, Jean (2000),
The Seeds of Speech: Language Origin and Evolution (Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press).
Chomsky, Noam (1972),
Language and the Mind (New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich).
Corballis, Michael C. (2002),
From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Deacon, Terrance (1997),
The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain(New York: W.W. Norton).
Elgin, Suzette H. (1973),
What is Linguistics? (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall).
Gitt, Werner (1999),
The Wonder of Man (Bielefeld, Germany: Christliche Literatur-Verbreitung E.V.).
Holloway, R.L. (1976), “Paleoneurological Evidence for Language Origins,”
Origins and Evolution of Language and Speech, ed. S.R. Harnad, D. Horst, D. Steklis, and J. Lancaster,
Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 280:330.
Horgan, John (1995), “A Sign is Born,”
Scientific American, 273[6]:18-19, December.
Jones, Steve, Robert Martin, and David Pilbeam, eds. (1999),
Cambridge Encyclopedia of Human Evolution (New York: Cambridge University Press).
Lemonick, Michael D. (1994), “How Man Began,”
Time, 143[11]:80-87, March 14.
Liebereman, Philip (1997), “Peak Capacity,”
The Sciences, 37:27, November/December.
Lieberman, Philip (1998),
Eve Spoke: Human Language and Human Evolution (New York: W.W. Norton).
Lyons, Eric and Bert Thompson (2002), “In the ‘Image and Likeness of God,’ ”
Reason & Revelation [Parts I and II], 22:17-23,25-31, March and April.
Matthews, Stephen, Bernard Comrie, and Marcia Polinsky, eds. (1996),
Atlas of Languages: The Origin and Development of Languages Throughout the World (New York: Facts on File).
McCrone, John (1991),
The Ape That Spoke: Language and the Evolution of the Human Mind (New York: William Morrow).
Moore, John N. (1983),
How to Teach Origins Without ACLU Interference (Milford, MI: Mott Media).
Nottebohm, F. (1980), “Testosterone Triggers Growth of Brain Vocal Control Nuclei in Adult Female Canaries,”
Brain Research, 189:429–436.
Nowak, Martin A. and David C. Krakauer (1999), “The Evolution of Language,”
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 96:8028-8033, July 6.
Nowak, Martin A., N.L. Komarova, and P. Niyogi (2001), “Evolution of Universal Grammar,”
Science, 291:114-118, January 5.
Pinker, Steven (1997a),
How the Mind Works (New York: W.W. Norton).
Pinker, Steven (1997b),
The Language Instinct: The New Science of Language and Mind (London: Penguin).
Ross, Phillip E. (1991), “Hard Words,”
Scientific American, 264[4]:138-147, April.
Savage-Rumbaugh, Sue and Roger Lewin (1994), “Ape at the Brink,”
Discover, 15[9]:90-96,98.
Simpson, George Gaylord (1966), “The Biological Nature of Man,”
Science, 152:467-477, April 22.
Skoyles, John R. and Dorion Sagan (2002),
Up from Dragons (New York: McGraw-Hill).
Thomas, Lewis (1980), “On Science and Uncertainty,”
Discover, 1:59, October.
Wieland, Carl (1999), “Towering Change,”
Creation Ex Nihilo, 22[1]:22-26, December 1999– February 2000.
Zimmer, Carl (2001),
Evolution (New York: HarperCollins).